Discussion about this post

User's avatar
Jen O’Sullivan's avatar

Thank you so much for your thoughtful comment, Laura. I'm really glad the piece resonated with you. You’re absolutely right to highlight the confusion that often surrounds teacher education and teacher CPD in this area. Many teachers enter the profession with fragmented or even contradictory messages about dyslexia, so it’s no wonder that uncertainty persists.

You’ve touched on an important example with the age-six threshold. While it’s true that a formal diagnosis of dyslexia is typically not made before age six or seven (if a child is lucky!), that doesn’t mean we can’t identify children who are at risk much earlier. We now have tools that can flag early indicators (like poor phonological awareness or difficulty learning letter-sound correspondences) in the first years of school, allowing us to intervene earlier and more effectively.

Thanks again for engaging with the post.

Expand full comment
David Hammond's avatar

I'm a bit late to the party on this post but I thought I would offer a perspective. Before I comment further, I would add some context to my statements. I live in New Zealand, I'm very dyslexic and dyspraxic, and despite the best efforts of the New Zealand education system in the 1980s and 1990's I eventually ended up with a masters degree from the London School of Economics of Economics.

Firstly, dyslexia cuts across multiple academic disciplines and this comes with different perspectives. A prime example might be the difference between an educationalist and an anthropologist's view of dyslexia (though you could just as easily throw geneticists and neuroscientists into the mix as comparators).

Neither is wrong but they are very different perspectives; one has a very strong focus on the here and now of literacy, the other places it within the continuum of human existence and the fact that for most of human existence reading was something only a small proportion of the population did, which leads to an obvious question about why is there this proportion of the population that struggles with literacy acquisition when for the longest time literacy acquisition was not a normal part of the human lifecycle, could there be another reason for this difference in language processing (I use language processing deliberately because this takes account of the challenges some dyslexics have with working memory, and processing speed)?

Secondly, as someone who's working memory scored on the lowest part of the 1st percentile for some of the working memory assessments in dyslexia testing, this can't be under estimated in the impact it has on academic performance and wider life (though it barely seems to feature in the academic discourse of educators when discussing dyslexia).

Thirdly, I think the superpower narrative came out as push back against the many negative myths surrounding dyslexia (I think potential strengths might be more a useful phrase, but when you are struggling in the classroom achieving in some other part of life can do wonders for motivation and self-esteem that can overflow into the classroom). It's true there isn't much academic research to backup this idea, save for Dr Juile Logan's research into entrepreneurship and Dr Helen Taylor's work in placing dyslexia within an evolutionary context.

But its also important to note that most of the focus and funding for dyslexia research has focused on the deficits, if there had been just as much research on potential strengths we might have a more definitive answer one way or the other. I do know that someone like Dr Helen Taylor would argue that in some of the studies that show no strengths in dyslexia, there are potentially methodological flaws in those studies that limit their ability to assess and measure possible dyslexic strengths. I think it's an area of research in its infancy, not its maturity.

Expand full comment
1 more comment...

No posts